Shamsuddeen Nasir Shehu1, Sadiya Tahir2, Abdullahi Dahiru3, Abdulmalik A. Lawan4*
1Department of Clinical Services, Federal Neuropsychiatric Hospital, Kware, Sokoto Nigeria
2Department of Medicine, Murtala Muhammad Specialist Hospital, Kano Nigeria
3College of Science and Midwifery Kano, School of Nursing, Kano Nigeria
4Department of Computer Science, Kano University of Science and Technology, Kano Nigeria
*Corresponding Author’s Email: aalawan@kustwudil.edu.ng
ABSTRACT
Definitive explanations on the associations between demographics and cause as well as the cure of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) are yet to be known due to the unavailability of universal datasets and cost-effective diagnostic measures. This study analyzed large ASD screening data to examine whether symptoms of ASD differ based on ethnicity. The result showed a significant difference in the Autism Quotient (AQ) scores based on ethnicity among children, adolescents, and adults. Higher internal consistency was recorded on self-reported cases. This study will advance understanding of the influence of demographics on ASD symptoms. It is suggested that future studies should improve the reliability of AQ as screening tool.
INTRODUCTION
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that was initially misconceived to be prevalent only in western countries. The influence of demographics on prevalence is among the early misconceptions about autism. Due to the inaccessibility of screening instruments, a significant number of studies focused on regional data to describe the influence of demographic differences on the symptoms of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, researchers and practitioners proposed numerous screening instruments and practices to mitigate the drawbacks of current ASD diagnostic procedures. ASD screening is a quick and preliminary practice of determining people “at-risk” of ASD for a referral to the rigorous diagnostic procedures. Among the popular screening instruments, studies have employed versions of Autism Quotient (AQ) to numerically score the symptoms of ASD in children (Auyeung et al., 2008), adolescents (Baron-cohen et al., 2006) and adults (Woodbury-Smith et al., 2005). In this study, we analyzed three large datasets in drawing vital conclusions on the effects of the demographic factor of ethnicity on the symptoms of ASD based on AQ scores.
Literature Review
Currently, little is known about the effect of demographics on the symptoms of ASD and this impedes vital generalizable assertions on symptomatology and epidemiology of autism spectrum disorder. Significant research has been conducted on the influence of assorted demographic factors on ASD (Baron-cohen et al., 2006; Frazier et al., 2014; Rosenberg et al., 2018; Sedgewick, Leppanen, & Tchanturia, 2019; Tillmann et al., 2018; Woodbury-Smith et al., 2005). Although previous studies investigated the influence of ethnicity on ASD (Keen, Reid and Arnone, 2010; Dealberto, 2011; Zaroff and Uhm, 2012; Becerra et al., 2014; Pierce et al., 2014; Harrison et al., 2017; Yingling, Hock and Bell, 2018; Abdullahi et al., 2019; Nevison and Zahorodny, 2019; Yingling, Bell and Hock, 2019); there is less evidence on the differences in the symptoms of ASD between ethnicities from a large dataset of children, adolescents and adults. For instance, Dealberto (2011) reviewed of related studies and laments the complexity in the relationship between ethnicity and ASD. Similarly, Zaroff and Uhm (2012) conducted a comprehensive literature review by comparing data across the assorted studies with particular focus on ethnicity and suggested that methodological factors are largely responsible for the differences in ASD prevalence across studies. Pierce et al. (2014) highlighted that ethnicity has not been adequately reported based on the review of three autism-related journals over a 6-year period.
Keen, Reid, and Arnone (2010) conducted a retrospective case-note analysis on 428 children diagnosed with ASD over a period of 6-years to examine the link between maternal ethnicity and the rate of childhood ASD. The result showed higher risk of ASD among children of mothers born outside Europe and mothers of Black ethnicity had a significantly higher risk compared with White mothers. Becerra et al. (2014) investigated the presence of significant difference in ASD phenotypes based on maternal ethnicity from a large sample of children diagnosed with ASD. The result indicated a significant association of maternal ethnicity with child’s and its severity. Harrison et al. (2017) employed measurement noninvariance in examining the diagnostic accuracy of Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) among 2458 culturally-diverse participants and found significant item-level bias based on ethnicity.
Recently, Nevison and Zahorodny (2019) analysed a decade of U.S birth cohorts of children with ASD in tracking time trends in the prevalence of ASD based on ethnicity and found various fluctuations between the racial groups. Similarly, Abdullahi et al. (2019) analysed the Western Australian Register of 4776 children diagnosed with ASD from 1999 to 2017 and found that children born to immigrant mothers had an increased risk of ASD symptoms.
However, further investigation on how ASD symptoms vary across ethnicities could help in understanding the possible need to redesign how ASD should be defined and diagnosed based on demographic variations (Abdullahi et al., 2019; Evans et al., 2018; Nevison & Zahorodny, 2019).
The Research Question
A definitive explanation of the associations between ethnicity and prevalence of ASD remains unknown. While some studies relatively maintained the aging assumptions of its higher prevalence in western countries (Becerra et al., 2014; Kogan et al., 2008; Palmer et al., 2010; Rosenberg et al., 2009), others aligned the increase to accessibility of effective diagnostic tools and practices (Begeer et al., 2009; Canino et al., 2010; Honda et al. 2005). Although studies identified the significance of ethnicity as a risk factor for ASD (Keen, Reid and Arnone, 2010; Dealberto, 2011; Becerra et al., 2014), the omission of a balanced ethnicity data in previous ASD studies impedes comparative analyses on its prevalence rates across ethnic groups (Zaroff and Uhm, 2012). The main research question of the study will examine whether there is a significant difference in ASD scores based on ethnicity in the different datasets (children, adolescents, adults and combined datasets).
METHODOLOGY
Participants
The voluntary 1875 participants were drawn from various ethnicities, and countries. Cross tabulations shown in Tables 1 and 2 described the demographic splits of the participants in children, adolescents, and adults screening categories (datasets).
Data Collection Tool
Three distinct open-source datasets for children, adolescents, and adults screened for “at-risk” of ASD were obtained from a mobile-based ASD screening study conducted by Thabtah (2019). In addition to demographic data, each of the datasets captured ten responses based on AQ-children, AQ-adolescents, and AQ-adults for screening children, adolescents, and adults respectively. Thabtah (2019) discussed the development, scoring, and validity of the questions contained in AQ-children, AQ-adolescents, and AQ-adults. The responses were rated on a 2-point scale with “Strongly Agree” and “Agree” assigned value 1 while others assigned value 0. The reliability scale of the items’ response yielded a Cronbach’s Alpha values of .639, .677, and .712, for children, adolescents, and adults respectively while .692 was recorded as the overall Cronbach’s Alpha as shown in Table 3.
It worth noting that, the research viability of autism quotient is limited by its lack of subscales; yielding only one reliability score. Thus, in the present study reliability scores for the screening categories were calculated as well as the overall. Higher internal consistency (α=.712, Table 3), higher “self-reported cases” (n=920, Table 4), and higher incidence rate of “at-risk” cases (n= 358, Table 5) were recorded on adults’ screening category. These findings provide significant indicators of how less reliable third-party responses (by parents, carers, or clinicians) might be on the true symptoms of ASD. In other words, self-reported cases have higher consistency than third-party responses.
Data Analysis
The datasets were organized and analyzed using SPSS 22. After a thorough check on data quality, and the reliability of the items, statistical techniques were employed in examining the research question. In the analysis on the ethnicity as categorical a data and ASD score provides continuous data, after conducting normality tests on the categories, it was found that basic conditions for applying parametric tests were not satisfied by the data (Harwell, 1988; McKight and Najab, 2010; Siegel, 2012). Thus, the effect of ethnicity on ASD scores in each dataset and the combined datasets was estimated by employing the k-independent samples test of Kruskal Wallis.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
We employed descriptive statistics to determine whether there is enough statistical evidence in favor of differences in ASD scores across the participants based on ethnicity. The combined dataset indicated the wide range between the numbers of observations in each screening category (Table 1-5); not normally distributed and not fit for parametric techniques (Harwell, 1988; McKight and Najab, 2010; Siegel, 2012). Thus, as stated earlier Kruskal Wallis is the most appropriate statistical technique to used. Based on the results found as shown in Tables 7-10 and summarized in Table 6, “there is a significant difference in ASD scores based on ethnicity in every the dataset (children, adolescents and adults datasets) as well as the combined dataset.
Screening Category | Total | ||||
Children | Adolescence | Adult | |||
Gender | Male | 363 | 117 | 596 | 1076 |
Female | 146 | 131 | 522 | 799 | |
Total | 509 | 248 | 1118 | 1875 |
Screening Category | Total | ||||
Children | Adolescence | Adult | |||
Ethnicity | Aboriginal | 6 | 3 | 21 | 30 |
Asian | 99 | 24 | 162 | 285 | |
Black | 25 | 20 | 58 | 103 | |
Hispanic | 17 | 10 | 23 | 50 | |
Latino | 16 | 8 | 36 | 60 | |
Middle eastern | 107 | 46 | 210 | 363 | |
South Asians | 23 | 3 | 39 | 65 | |
White | 198 | 118 | 524 | 840 | |
Others | 18 | 16 | 44 | 78 | |
Total | 509 | 248 | 1117 | 1874 |
Category | Number of cases | Number of items | Cronbach’s Alpha |
Children | 509 | 10 items | .639 |
Adolescences | 248 | 10 items | .677 |
Adults | 1118 | 10 items | .712 |
Overall | 1875 | 10 items | .692 |
Screening Category | Total | ||||
Children | Adolescence | Adult | |||
User | Friend | 2 | 0 | 3 | 5 |
Healthcare professional | 21 | 18 | 10 | 49 | |
Others | 0 | 9 | 15 | 24 | |
Parent | 438 | 103 | 126 | 667 | |
Relative | 29 | 19 | 43 | 91 | |
Self | 17 | 99 | 920 | 1036 | |
Teacher | 2 | 0 | 1 | 3 | |
Total | 509 | 248 | 1118 | 1875 |
Screening Category | Total | ||||
Children | Adolescence | Adult | |||
ASD Class | Yes | 257 | 127 | 358 | 742 |
No | 252 | 121 | 760 | 1133 | |
Total | 509 | 248 | 1118 | 1875 |
Factor | Dataset | |||
Children | Adolescents | Adults | Combined | |
Ethnicity | √ | √ | √ | √ |
√ = There is a significant difference in ASD scores based on ethnicity
N= 509 | Frequency | Percentage | Median | Mean ranks | α | |
Ethnicity | Aboriginal | 6 | 1.2 | 7.0000 | 288.58 | 0.000 |
Asian | 99 | 19.4 | 6.0000 | 246.02 | ||
Black | 25 | 4.9 | 7.0000 | 271.04 | ||
Hispanic | 17 | 3.3 | 7.0000 | 292.09 | ||
Latino | 16 | 3.1 | 7.0000 | 285.56 | ||
Middle eastern | 107 | 21.0 | 5.0000 | 197.40 | ||
South Asians | 23 | 4.5 | 6.0000 | 227.37 | ||
White | 198 | 38.9 | 7.0000 | 288.27 | ||
Others | 18 | 3.5 | 5.0000 | 220.50 |
N= 248 | Frequency | Percentage | Median | Mean ranks | α | |
Ethnicity | Aboriginal | 3 | 1.2 | 4.0000 | 120.33 | 0.000 |
Asian | 24 | 9.7 | 7.0000 | 139.94 | ||
Black | 20 | 8.1 | 5.0000 | 91.23 | ||
Hispanic | 10 | 4.0 | 5.0000 | 113.95 | ||
Latino | 8 | 3.2 | 5.0000 | 110.25 | ||
Middle eastern | 46 | 18.5 | 4.0000 | 83.68 | ||
South Asians | 3 | 1.2 | 7.0000 | 142.83 | ||
White | 118 | 47.6 | 7.0000 | 141.08 | ||
Others | 16 | 6.5 | 7.0000 | 149.06 |
N= 1118 | Frequency | Percentage | Median | Mean ranks | α | |
Ethnicity | Aboriginal | 21 | 1.9 | 5.0000 | 539.14 | 0.000 |
Asian | 162 | 14.5 | 4.0000 | 464.71 | ||
Black | 58 | 5.2 | 6.0000 | 652.55 | ||
Hispanic | 23 | 2.1 | 5.0000 | 571.76 | ||
Latino | 36 | 3.2 | 7.0000 | 732.17 | ||
Middle eastern | 210 | 18.8 | 4.0000 | 401.37 | ||
South Asians | 39 | 3.5 | 4.0000 | 377.12 | ||
White | 524 | 46.9 | 6.0000 | 641.26 | ||
Others | 44 | 3.9 | 5.0000 | 577.85 |
N= 1875 | Frequency | Percentage | Median | Mean ranks | α | |
Ethnicity | Aboriginal | 30 | 1.6 | 5.00 | 909.62 | 0.000 |
Asian | 285 | 15.2 | 5.00 | 852.92 | ||
Black | 103 | 5.5 | 6.00 | 1001.41 | ||
Hispanic | 50 | 2.7 | 6.00 | 993.86 | ||
Latino | 60 | 3.2 | 7.00 | 1118.04 | ||
Middle eastern | 363 | 19.4 | 4.00 | 690.24 | ||
South Asians | 65 | 3.5 | 5.00 | 738.02 | ||
White | 840 | 44.8 | 6.50 | 1062.43 | ||
Others | 78 | 4.2 | 5.50 | 969.44 |
Based on the results depicted on ethnicities in each of the tables 7-10, it can be concluded that the symptoms of ASD measured based on numerical values of ASD scores (i.e. Median and Mean rank) are statistically significantly higher in the groups having higher scores and vice versa. In other words, the group having the highest ASD scores indicates the ethnicity to be considered as manifesting more symptoms of ASD.
The results depicted in Table 7 includes differences in ASD Scores based on Ethnicity from the Children dataset. The differences are statistically significant (α<0.001) with each of Aboriginals, Blacks, Hispanics, Latinos, and Whites recorded the highest median scores of 7.0 and Middle Eastern recorded the lowest median score of 5.0. Considering the results from Table 8, Asians, South Asians, and Whites have the highest median scores of 7.0 each, while each of Aboriginals and Middle Eastern recorded the lowest scores of 4.0. The differences recorded from the Adolescents dataset as depicted in Table 8 are statistically significant (α<0.001). Furthermore, Table 9 shows differences in ASD Scores based on Ethnicity from the Adults dataset. The differences are statistically significant (α<0.001) with Latino having the highest score (Median=7.0) and each of Asians, Middle Eastern, and South Asians recorded the lowest scores (Median=4.0).
Taking the three datasets as a whole, Table 10 and Figure 1 indicated that Latino recorded the highest ASD score with Mean rank of 1118.04 and Median of 7.0, followed by White (Mean rank=1062.43, Median=6.5), Black (Mean rank=1001.41, Median=6.0) and Hispanic (Mean rank= 993.86, Median=6.0). While Middle Eastern recorded the lowest score (Mean rank=690.24, Median=4.0) followed by South Asians (Mean rank=738.02, Median=5.0), Asian (Mean rank=852.92, Median=5.0) and Aboriginal (Mean rank=909.62, Median=5.0). The significance level (α<0.0001) indicated that the differences in ASD scores based on Ethnicity as depicted in Table 10 and Figure 1 are statistically significant.
CONCLUSION
Previous studies investigated the influence of ethnicity on the symptoms of ASD from samples of either children, adolescents, adults, or uncategorized samples. This study examined differences in Autism Quotient scores based on ethnicity from large samples of children, adolescents, and adults screened for at-risk of ASD. The results indicated the significant influence of ethnicity on ASD symptoms in children, adolescents as well as adults. Adults’ responses, most of which are self-ratings were found to have higher internal consistency based on the computed Cronbach’s Alpha; an indicator that self-rating are much reliable than third-party ratings in determining the true symptoms of ASD. Further research on the influence of demographics on ASD symptoms will aid in clearing the misconceptions about the prevalence of ASD. Definitive explanations on the association between ethnicity and ASD requires standard and accessible screening instruments, as well as centralized data for comparative analyses. Future studies should employ Artificial Intelligence modeling in AQ scoring and address the biases in third-party ratings of ASD symptoms. There is also a need to further investigate the influence of other demographic, biological and environmental factors on the symptoms and prevalence of ASD.
Conflict of Interests
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
The authors will like to acknowledge and thank Professor Fadi Thabtah for providing the datasets used.
REFERENCE
Abdullahi, I., Wong, K., Bebbington, K., Mutch, R., de Klerk, N., Cherian, S., Downs, J., Leonard, H.M. & Glasson, E., (2019) ‘Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder According to Maternal-Race Ethnicity and Country of Birth: A Register-Based Study’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Springer US, 49(9), pp. 3611–3624.
Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S. & Allison, C. (2008) ‘The autism spectrum quotient: Children’s version (AQ-Child)’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), pp. 1230–1240.
Baron-Cohen, S., Hoekstra, R.A., Knickmeyer, R.C. & Wheelwright, S. (2006) ‘The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ)—Adolescent Version’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(3), pp. 343–350.
Becerra, T. A., von Ehrenstein, O.S., Heck , J.E., Olsen, J., Arah, O.A., Jeste, S.S., Rodriguez, M. & Ritz, R. (2014) ‘Autism spectrum disorders and race, ethnicity, and nativity: A population-based study’, Pediatrics, 134(1).
Begeer, S., El Bouk, S., Boussaid, W., Terwogt,, M.M. & Koot , H.M. (2009) ‘Underdiagnosis and referral bias of autism in ethnic minorities’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), pp. 142–148.
Begeer, S., El Bouk, S., Boussaid, W., Terwogt,, M.M. & Koot , H.M. (2009) ‘Underdiagnosis and referral bias of autism in ethnic minorities’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), pp. 142–148.
Canino, G., Polanczyk, G., Bauermeister, J.J., Rohde, L.A. & Frick, P.J. et al. (2010) ‘Does the prevalence of CD and ODD vary across cultures?’, Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 45(7), pp. 695–704.
Dealberto, M. J. (2011) ‘Prevalence of autism according to maternal immigrant status and ethnic origin’, Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 123(5), pp. 339–348.
Evans, S.C., Boan, A.D., Bradley, C. & Carpenter, L.A. (2018) ‘Sex/Gender Differences in Screening for Autism Spectrum Disorder: Implications for Evidence-Based Assessment’, Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. Routledge,, pp. 1–15.
Frazier, T.W., Youngstrom, E., Embacher, R.A., Hardan, A.Y., Constantino, J.N. Law, P.A., Findling, R. & Eng, C. (2014) ‘Demographic and clinical correlates of autism symptom domains and autism spectrum diagnosis’, Autism, 18(5), pp. 571–582.
Harrison, A.J., Long, K.A., Tommet , D.C. & Jones, R.N. (2017) ‘Examining the Role of Race, Ethnicity, and Gender on Social and Behavioral Ratings Within the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Springer US, 47(9), pp. 2770–2782.
Harwell, M. R. (1988) ‘Choosing Between Parametric and Nonparametric Tests’, Journal of Counseling and Development, 67(September), pp. 34–37.
Honda, H., Shimizu, Y., Imai, M. & Nitto, Y. (2005) ‘Cumulative incidence of childhood autism: A total population study of better accuracy and precision’, Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. Queen Mary, University of London, 47(1), pp. 10–18.
Keen, D. V., Reid, F. D. & Arnone, D. (2010) ‘Autism, ethnicity and maternal immigration’, British Journal of Psychiatry, 196(4), pp. 274–281. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.109.065490.
Kogan, M.D., Strickland, B.B., Blumberg, S.J., Singh, G.K., Perrin, J.M. & van Dyck, P.C. (2008) ‘A national profile of the health care experiences and family impact of autism spectrum disorder among children in the united states, 2005-2006’, Pediatrics, 122(6), pp. 2005–2006.
McKight, P. E. & Najab, J. (2010) ‘Kruskal-Wallis Test’, in The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., p. 2009.
Nevison, C. & Zahorodny, W. (2019) ‘Race/Ethnicity-Resolved Time Trends in United States ASD Prevalence Estimates from IDEA and ADDM’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Springer US, 49(12), pp. 4721– 4730.
Palmer, R. F., Walker, T., Mandell, D., Bayles, B. & Miller, C.S., (2010) ‘Explaining low rates of autism among hispanic schoolchildren in Texas’, American Journal of Public Health, 100(2), pp. 270–272.
Pierce, N.P., O'Reilly, M.F., Sorrells, A.M., Fragale, C.L., White, P.J., Aguilar, J.M. & Cole, H.A. et al. (2014) ‘Ethnicity reporting practices for empirical research in three autism-related journals’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(7), pp. 1507–1519.
Rosenberg, R.E., Law, J.K., Kaufmann, W., Daniels, A.M. & Law, P.A. (2009) ‘Trends in autism spectrum disorder diagnoses: 1994-2007’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(8), pp. 1099–1111.
Rosenberg, S.A., Moody , E.J., Lee, L., DiGuiseppi, C., Windham, G.C., Wiggins, L.D., Schieve, L.A., Ledbetter, C.M., Levy, S. E., Blaskey, L., Young , L., Bernal, P., Rosenberg, C.R. & Fallin, M.D. (2018) ‘Influence of family demographic factors on social communication questionnaire scores’, Autism Research, 11(5), pp. 695–706.
Sedgewick, F., Leppanen, J. & Tchanturia, K. (2019) ‘The Friendship Questionnaire, autism, and gender differences: a study revisited’, Molecular Autism. Molecular Autism, 10(1), p. 40.
Siegel, S. (2012) ‘Nonparametric Statistics’, The American Statistician. Taylor & Francis Group, 11(3), pp. 13–19.
Thabtah, F. (2019) ‘An accessible and efficient autism screening method for behavioural data and predictive analyses’, Health Informatics Journal, 25(4), pp. 1739–1755.
Tillmann, J. et al. (2018) ‘Evaluating Sex and Age Differences in ADI-R and ADOS Scores in a Large European Multi-site Sample of Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Springer US, 48(7), pp. 2490–2505.
Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S. & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005) ‘Screening adults for Asperger Syndrome using the AQ: A preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), pp. 331–335.
Yingling, M. E., Bell, B. A. & Hock, R. M. (2019) ‘Treatment Utilization Trajectories among Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Differences by Race-Ethnicity and Neighborhood’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Springer US, 49(5), pp. 2173–2183.
Yingling, M. E., Hock, R. M. & Bell, B. A. (2018) ‘Time-Lag Between Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder and Onset of Publicly-Funded Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention: Do Race–Ethnicity and Neighborhood Matter?’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Springer US, 48(2), pp. 561–571.
Zaroff, C. M. & Uhm, S. Y. (2012) ‘Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders and influence of country of measurement and ethnicity’, Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47(3), pp. 395–398.